The effect of heredity and environment on Learning | B.Ed Hons | UoB

·        Introduction:



Environment and heredity have been considered two key factors that influence learning. Learning environment is critical in sound teaching and learning process. Review of literature shows that Internal and external environment affect learning to a greater extent. Besides environment, heredity/genetic factor has also been found to have some influence on human behavior. This article briefly discusses these factors and their influence on learning.
Childhood learning and literacy in general is dependent upon both prerequisites in the individual and the environment where this individual is growing up. Individual variations in the acquisition of literacy can of course depend upon cognitive and verbal ability as well as specific language difficulties such as articulatory and perceptual phonetic factors. In addition to this home environment plays a decisive role in the development of linguistic competence. In a home where the parents read for the child and where children’s questions are encouraged and responded to in an elaborated way the chances for a successful reading and writing competence are much better than in a linguistically deprived environment. Genetic factors also are influential since parents with reading and writing problems often get children with the same type of difficulties. Naturally it also works the other way around so that parents who are excellent readers and writers both genetically transfer this and show their children all the joys of being part of the world of letters and books. Also in intellectually proficient families there are children at risk of developing reading and writing difficulties. The parents could be too occupied to notice the need of stimulation in their child. Resources of the school might be insufficient in areas where most of the children are expected to read and write without problems, and the child might feel different and stupid in comparison to the classmates. In addition to this there are substantial differences in physical maturation between boys and girls contributing to the boys having more difficulties in young ages to adapt to the traditional school system. This might increase the number of boys being referred to special education.

·         Influence of Environment on Learning

The behavior and development of people is mostly affected by the physical environment in which the live. School environment is vital in enhancing learning. A conducive environment of the school compound for instance, helps learners to be able to achieve well in their academic pursuits. The school environment can be viewed as the personality of the school which makes it distinct and unique from others.
A survey by Swedish Work Environment Authority (2005) investigated the environmental impact on effective instruction. The study found out that a good psycho-social school environment is an important prerequisite of learning and of pupil satisfaction in school. The study dealt specifically with environment and its impact on effective learning without looking into the overall components of effective learning.
The physical environment of the classroom also affects the individual children and the group as a whole. The physical environment includes the size of the room, the colors of the walls, the type of flooring, the amount of light, and the number of windows. A physical environment which is secure, appealing and well planned helps children to effectively engage in healthy learning activities. Such an environment can support goals for children and helps them to interact in positive ways, thus reinforcing learning.
If genetic influences account for between 40 and 80 percent of the variation in intelligence, then environmental influences account for between 20 and 60 percent of the total variation. Environmental factors comprise all the stimuli a person encounters from conception to death, including food, cultural information, education, and social experiences. Although it is known that environmental factors can be potent forces in shaping intelligence, it is not understood exactly how they contribute to intelligence. In fact, scientists have identified few specific environmental variables that have direct, unambiguous effects on intelligence. Many environmental variables have small effects and differ in their effect on each person, making them difficult to identify.
Schooling is an important factor that affects intelligence. Children who do not attend school or who attend intermittently score more poorly on IQ tests than those who attend regularly, and children who move from low-quality schools to high-quality schools tend to show improvements in IQ. Besides transmitting information to students directly, schools teach problem solving, abstract thinking, and how to sustain attention—all skills required on IQ tests.
Many researchers have investigated whether early intervention programs can prevent the lowered intelligence that may result from poverty or other disadvantaged environments. In the United States, Head Start is a federally funded preschool program for children from families whose income is below the poverty level. Head Start and similar programs in other countries attempt to provide children with activities that might enhance cognitive development, including reading books, learning the alphabet and the numbers, learning the names of colors, drawing, and other activities. These programs often have large initial effects on IQ scores. Children who participate gain as much as 15 IQ points compared to control groups of similar children not in the program. Unfortunately, these gains seem to last only as long as the intervention lasts. When children from these programs enter school, their IQ declines to the level of control groups over a period of several years. This has come to be known as the “fade-out” effect.
Even though early intervention preschool programs do not seem to produce lasting IQ gains, some studies suggest they may have other positive long-term effects. For example, the Consortium for Longitudinal Studies reported that participants are less likely to repeat grades, less likely be placed in remedial classes, and more likely to finish high school than comparable non-participants—even though both groups show about the same levels of academic achievement. Preschoolers in early intervention programs may also benefit from improved health and nutrition, and their mothers may sometimes benefit from additional education that the programs provide. Because a substantial portion of the variation in intelligence is due to environmental factors, early intervention programs should be able to produce significant and lasting IQ gains once the specific environmental variables that influence IQ have been identified. Researchers continue to search for the interventions that will increase IQ and, ultimately, academic achievement.

·         Influence of Heredity on Learning

Many aspects of human characteristics such as height and eye color are largely determined by genetics. Learning has also been tied with genetic influence.
In the 17th and 18th centuries, philosophers such as René Descartes and Immanuel Kant argued that human cognition was largely reflective of genetically determined predispositions. They maintained that humans are born with certain cognitive inclinations. On the contrary, John Locke compared the human mind to a piece of blank paper without any ideas written on it (tabula rasa), meaning blank slate. He suggested that only from experience do humans draw reason and knowledge. Following these opposed ideas, scholars have continued to explore the influence of heredity on learning.
A study by Frank et al (2007) on how genes influence learning is a source of new insights among scientist on the role of genetics in learning. The study found links to learning behaviors in three separate genes associated with dopamine (a chemical in the brain that is associated with pleasure, learning among other behaviors). The study found genetic differences between slow and fast learners. Such findings among others, have continued to address the issue of genetic influence on learning.
In behavioral genetics, the heritability of a trait refers to the proportion of the trait’s variation within a population that is attributable to genetics. The heritability of intelligence is usually defined as the proportion of the variation in IQ scores that is linked to genetic factors. To estimate the heritability of intelligence, scientists compare the IQs of individuals who have differing degrees of genetic relationship. Scientists have conducted hundreds of studies, involving tens of thousands of participants that have sought to measure the heritability of intelligence. The generally accepted conclusion from these studies is that genetic factors account for 40 to 80 percent of the variability in intelligence test scores, with most experts settling on a figure of approximately 50 percent. But heritability estimates apply only to populations and not to individuals. Therefore, one can never say what percentage of a specific individual’s intelligence is inherited based on group heritability’s alone.
Although any degree of genetic relationship can and has been studied, studies of twins are particularly informative. Identical twins develop from one egg and are genetically identical to each other. Fraternal twins develop from separate eggs and, like ordinary siblings, have only about half of their genes in common. Comparisons between identical and fraternal twins can be very useful in determining heritability. Scientists have found that the IQ scores of identical twins raised together are remarkably similar to each other, while those of fraternal twins are less similar to each other. This finding suggests a genetic influence in intelligence. Interestingly, fraternal twins’ IQ scores are more similar to each other than those of ordinary siblings, a finding that suggests environmental effects. Some researchers account for the difference by noting that fraternal twins are probably treated more alike than ordinary siblings because they are the same age.
Some of the strongest evidence for genetic influences in intelligence comes from studies of identical twins adopted into different homes early in life and thus raised in different environments. Identical twins are genetically identical, so any differences in their IQ scores must be due entirely too environmental differences and any similarities must be due to genetics. Results from these studies indicate that the IQ scores of identical twins raised apart are highly similar—nearly as similar as those of identical twins raised together. For adoption studies to be valid, placement of twin pairs must be random. If brighter twin pairs are selectively placed in the homes of adoptive parents with higher intelligence, it becomes impossible to separate genetic and environmental influences.
Another way of studying the genetic contribution to intelligence is through adoption studies, in which researchers compare adopted children to their biological and adoptive families. Adopted children have no genetic relationship to their adoptive parents or to their adoptive parents’ biological children. Thus, any similarity in IQ between the adopted children and their adoptive parents or the parents’ biological children must be due to the similarity of the environment they all live in, and not to genetics.
There are two interesting findings from studies of adopted children. First, the IQs of adopted children have only a small relationship to the IQs of their adoptive parents and the parents’ biological children. Second, after the adopted child leaves home, this small relationship becomes smaller. In general, the IQs of adopted children are always more similar to their biological parents’ IQs than to their adoptive parents’ IQs. Further, once they leave the influence of their adoptive home, they become even more similar to their biological parents. Both of these findings suggest the importance of hereditary factors in intelligence.
People sometimes assume that if intelligence is highly heritable, then it cannot be changed or improved through environmental factors. This assumption is incorrect. For example, height has very high heritability, yet average heights have increased in the 20th century among the populations of many industrialized nations, most likely because of improved nutrition and health care. Similarly, performance on IQ tests has increased with each generation, yet few scientists attribute this phenomenon to genetic changes. Thus, many experts believe that improved environments can, to some degree, increase a person’s intelligence.
Some genetic disorders, such as phenylketonuria (PKU) and Down syndrome, may result in mental retardation and low IQ. But evidence for genetic influences should not be interpreted as evidence of a direct connection between genes and intelligence. In PKU, for example, a rare combination of recessive genes sets the stage for a series of biochemical interactions that ultimately results in low IQ. These interactions only occur, however, in the presence of the amino acid phenylalanine. If the disorder is detected early and phenylalanine is withheld from the infant’s diet, then large IQ deficits do not develop.

·        How Does Heredity and Environment Influence on learning and intelligence?

There are many claims, opinions, and arguments about where intelligence comes from - is it from our genetic heritage, or is it a consequence of the environment and our experiences? Below are some research findings and compelling arguments about how our genes and our society influence intelligence.

·        Genetic Influences on learning and Intelligence

Research findings confirming that intelligence is a by-product of our heredity revolves around finding specific genetic markers and associating IQ levels of parents and siblings.
Genetic markers on intelligence are believed to be found in chromosomes 4, 6 and 22. The genetic marker in chromosome 6 appears in 1/3 of children with high IQ, and only 1/6 of children with average IQs.

·        Environmental Influences on learning & Intelligence

Environmental influences come in different packages - family, ethnicity, socio-economic status, gender, and education.
The family has a critical role in elevating IQ scores. Communication received during the first 3 years of life is the primary predictor of Stanford-Binet IQ score at age 3. Moreover, simply moving or exposing children to families with better environments increases their IQs by 12 points.
The notion of intelligence also differs across cultures. Socio-economic status is also very important. Having the means and the resources gives a sense of security and access to stimulating environments.
One's gender can also influence intelligence. On average, males and females have similar IQ levels. However, males' scores are more variable. Males specifically perform better in spatial and abstract reasoning, while females tend to be better at finding synonyms.
Lastly, and perhaps the most important of all, is education. Intervention day care increases IQ score by 17 points at age 3, and 5 points by age 15. South African Indian children lost 5 IQ points per year of schooling delayed due to the unavailability of teachers. The gap between African and White Americans on standardized test scores is narrowing, especially in college, most likely because of having similar educational environments. In 1999, James Flynn also discovered that the human race is becoming more and more intelligent (also called the Flynn Effect). In the ancient times, only adult priests can read; nowadays, 7-year old children must already know how to read.

·        Variation in early language and literacy development

Children acquire language proficiency in communication with the environment. Language development is thus linked to both a cognitive and a socio-cultural perspective. Language learning is an adaptive process consisting of biological prerequisites to process information as well as the communication patterns and language structures that the child encounters. The child stores information and adapts to new stimuli by reorganizing and adapting. This is however a reciprocal process since the social environment also adapts to the child. When the child begins to talk, the child possesses a powerful tool that can be used in influencing the social environment. Speech starts with an increase in vocabulary which is followed by morphology and grammar in typically developing children. This process is dependent upon the social environment of the child but does not require specific training. Literacy on the other hand does not occur by itself but has to be taught in an educational process. It is therefore not self-evident that children who are orally advanced are not encountering difficulties in learning to read and write. On the other hand children who are orally advanced can show profound difficulties learning to read.
The variation in language development is therefore dependent upon perceptual, motor and cognitive abilities relevant for language learning but also on the interplay between the child and its social and educational environment.

·        Conclusion:

Today researchers agree that heredity and environment have an interactive influence on intelligence. Heredity places an upper and lower limit on the IQ that can be attained by a given person. The environment determines where within these limits the person’s IQ will lie. If the environmental factors were more important than the genetic factors the defense would have been very high. Adoption studies also show that adopted children somewhat resemble their biological parents in intelligence researchers have found that IQ scores of adopted children more highly correlation with that of their biological parents than that of their adaptive parent’s IQ.
Because of strong genetic link between the adopted children and their biological parents it can be concluded that heredity is more important than environment. Researchers have also found that heritability of intelligence increases from as low as 0.45, in infancy to as high as 0.80 in late adulthood. This is because as we grow older, the effect of environment reduces upon us and we interact more with the environment as we like not under our parent’s pressure or the of society.
Example: Sometimes parents push children to become engineers / doctors, incompatible environments, but as adults these individuals may select their own career environments.
The heritability index has several flaws as it greatly depends on the data fed into its calculation. The data is collected from traditional IQ tests which is deemed by some experts as not the best indicator of intelligence. Also heritability assumes that both environment and heredity are separate factors and each excerpts a distinct amount of influence but genes and environment works together, with the environment shaping the genetic activity. Children are not predestined to have an intelligence level similar to their biological parents.
Example: Most children of extra ordinary intelligence were born to average intelligence parents. Genetic ancestry of a child is not a 100 % or surefire predictor of his / her intelligence environment also makes an appreciable difference as we shall now see.
Today most researchers agree that modifications in environment can change the IQ scores of individuals considerably. Biological unrelated children raised together in the same home have some similarity in IQ. A correlation of 0.25 exists for unrelated children reared together in the same environment moreover, blacks who are raised in economically enriched environments have IQ score similar to whites in comparable environments.
Example: in a study of black children adopted by middle class while families at an early, the IQ of the adopted children average 15 point above the IQ scores of un-adopted black children the racial gap in IQ narrows considerably after college education.
Early childhood literacy development is dependent upon both hereditary and other individual prerequisites as well as influences in the home, peer group, school and society. An interactional model based on results from twin research illustrates the importance of restrictive and stimulating factors in the environment at various levels. A permissive setting tends to increase the literacy variation depending upon heredity and home background influences.
From the research regarding heredity-environment influences on early childhood literacy it can be concluded that genetic aspects are evident and particularly so for children at risk for developing reading and writing difficulties. Phonological awareness seems to be a key factor in this respect. Children who have difficulties combining sounds and letters need structured and encouraging reading instruction by preschool in order to protect them from school failure later on. Parents are both transferring their genes to their children and a home environment where reading and writing might be more or less encouraged.


*****

Post a Comment

Previous Post Next Post

نموذج الاتصال